We read with interest the article by Harper et al in the last edition
of this journal (2011;1:306-309). By establishing a correlation between
infant mortality and the increased mortality risk of the parents, the
article speaks, albeit indirectly, to the possible links between emotional
and physical states - a topic of important and growing interest. However,
two points present themselves about the authors' analysis - the f...
We read with interest the article by Harper et al in the last edition
of this journal (2011;1:306-309). By establishing a correlation between
infant mortality and the increased mortality risk of the parents, the
article speaks, albeit indirectly, to the possible links between emotional
and physical states - a topic of important and growing interest. However,
two points present themselves about the authors' analysis - the first is
statistical, the second conceptual.
Harper et al are direct about possible confounding factors for which
they were unable to control but which may nullify any causal link implied
by the statistical correlation they identify (p308). However, given their
use of census data, there is one possible confounding factor that they
might have controlled for - age of parent at the time of birth.
Harper et al imply that the small difference in mean age between the
comparison and control group means age should not raise concern. However,
if parent and infant mortality are more likely among both older and
younger parents, the difference in mean age might be minimal or invisible,
even though the casual implications of the chi-squared correlation would
be weakened. Previous research suggests that such a two-sided confounding
factor may exist for age. Young births, such as teenage pregnancies, have
been linked to worse outcomes for both mother and child (see, for example,
citations in Wellings and Kaye, 1999). Births to older parents are
similarly more likely to face complications and older parents would
naturally be closer to average life expectancies.
The results presented by the authors point towards this possibility.
The 'bereaved' group manifests a wider range of ages than the 'control'
group. The effect is most significant in the English data - the smallest
difference in standard deviations exists for the 1981-1991 cohort, which
is between 8.4 and 5.7. Since the English data manifest a higher relative
risk, it is worth exploring whether age may be such a confounding factor.
Year of birth might also be included in the analysis, to control for the
possibility that both infant and parent mortality risks may be shifting
over time.
The second concern is a conceptual one. If a ceteris paribus causal
link exists between the death of one's children and an increased mortality
risk for parents, it seems likely that this is linked to the trauma caused
by the loss of a loved one. But emotional bonds are not all equal - on
average, they might be expected to become stronger over time. From this
perspective, the average relative risk of parent mortality after a
miscarriage should be lower than death in childbirth, which would be lower
in turn than death in early infancy and the death of a young child.
The Dutch study (Li et al, 2003) referenced by Harper et al bears out
this principle - the relative risk of parent mortality in their study is
higher for a child dying aged between 1 and 11 months than a child dying
at less than one month. The analysis by Harper et al can be extended to
explore this hypothesis. Parents suffering stillbirths and infant
mortalities are currently combined into a single category - by splitting
the two groups, differences might be observed. The accidental inclusion of
parents whose child dies aged older than one in the 'non-bereaved' group
would bias effects downward, but differences may still be observable.
For the Scottish data, ethical non-identification reasons and small
sample sizes prevented such subgroup analysis (p307). However, the small
sample size resulted by design - the choice of 20 birthdays or 5.3% of the
population. Had the authors chosen a larger set of birthdays, it may have
been ethically possible to analyse the two subgroups in greater detail. No
such restriction applied to the English data.
The authors are to be congratulated on their research and we agree
with them that further statistical and case study work is required. We
hope that the suggestions are amenable to straightforward exploration on
similar datasets to those used by the authors.
Dr Wing Chu, ST1 Doctor in Palliative Care; Christian Percy,
Statistical Consultant
References
Wellings, K. and Kane, R. (1999), 'Trends in teenage pregnancy in England
and Wales: how can we explain them', J R Soc Med 1999;92:277-282
We read with interest the article by Harper et al in the last edition of this journal (2011;1:306-309). By establishing a correlation between infant mortality and the increased mortality risk of the parents, the article speaks, albeit indirectly, to the possible links between emotional and physical states - a topic of important and growing interest. However, two points present themselves about the authors' analysis - the f...
Pages